Please visit and LIKE our Facebook Page and share with family, friends and on your own Facebook page, and ask them to share further – it is only by working together and sharing knowledge and education that we can improve the lives of dogs and assist owners. We do not inundate you with posts – an average of 5 per week, and the odd Did You Know. Thank You!
Did you know that the manner you train can affect how secure your dog feels?
www.avsab.org and originally in Companion Animal Psychology
www.avsab.org and originally in Companion Animal Psychology
Dogs trained with aversive methods are less likely to show a ‘secure base’ effect, study shows.
When people use aversive methods to train dogs, it is associated with risks to the dog’s welfare, including fear, stress, and aggression (Ziv 2017; Guilherme-Fernandes et al 2017). Research published last year in Applied Animal Behaviour Science by Dr. Ana Vieira de Castro (Universidade do Porto) suggests it also affects the dog’s relationship with their owner. By Zazie Todd, PhD
Aversive dog training methods are those based on positive punishment and negative reinforcement. The schools in this study that were classed as using aversive methods often used jerks on the leash, yelling at the dog, hitting the dog (all examples of positive punishment), or pulling the dog’s leash to choke their collar until they sat down (negative reinforcement).
Reward-based methods (positive reinforcement and negative punishment) are recommended by many dog training and veterinary associations because they work and they have fewer risks. The reward-based schools in this study did not use any of the methods mentioned in the previous paragraph. For all six of the schools, the scientists analyzed video of classes to assess the type of training methods.
The study used something called the Ainsworth Strange Situation Test to look at the attachment between dogs and their owners. This test has been used extensively with infants to study their attachment to their parents, and has also been used with dogs.
The test involves a series of separations and reunions between the dog and their owner and a stranger. Because previous research has shown that the order of these separations may matter, the study used two different orders in a counterbalanced design.
The study looked at three aspects of the dog’s attachment to their owner. One is the secure base effect, whereby the owner is a secure base from which the dog can explore. (Similarly, for infants that are securely attached, the parent is a secure base to explore from, and a safe haven to return to if something is stressful).
These results show that there was a secure base effect for the dogs that had been trained with reward-based methods. Comparing these results to previous research on attachment in dogs, the scientists write, When dogs had been trained with reward-based methods, they were more likely to play when the owner was in the room compared to when the stranger was in the room. The dogs in the reward-based group spent more time exploring the room in the presence of the owner compared to when the stranger was there. These dogs also engaged in more greeting behaviours when the owner came back into the room.
When people use aversive methods to train dogs, it is associated with risks to the dog’s welfare, including fear, stress, and aggression (Ziv 2017; Guilherme-Fernandes et al 2017). Research published last year in Applied Animal Behaviour Science by Dr. Ana Vieira de Castro (Universidade do Porto) suggests it also affects the dog’s relationship with their owner. By Zazie Todd, PhD
Aversive dog training methods are those based on positive punishment and negative reinforcement. The schools in this study that were classed as using aversive methods often used jerks on the leash, yelling at the dog, hitting the dog (all examples of positive punishment), or pulling the dog’s leash to choke their collar until they sat down (negative reinforcement).
Reward-based methods (positive reinforcement and negative punishment) are recommended by many dog training and veterinary associations because they work and they have fewer risks. The reward-based schools in this study did not use any of the methods mentioned in the previous paragraph. For all six of the schools, the scientists analyzed video of classes to assess the type of training methods.
The study used something called the Ainsworth Strange Situation Test to look at the attachment between dogs and their owners. This test has been used extensively with infants to study their attachment to their parents, and has also been used with dogs.
The test involves a series of separations and reunions between the dog and their owner and a stranger. Because previous research has shown that the order of these separations may matter, the study used two different orders in a counterbalanced design.
The study looked at three aspects of the dog’s attachment to their owner. One is the secure base effect, whereby the owner is a secure base from which the dog can explore. (Similarly, for infants that are securely attached, the parent is a secure base to explore from, and a safe haven to return to if something is stressful).
These results show that there was a secure base effect for the dogs that had been trained with reward-based methods. Comparing these results to previous research on attachment in dogs, the scientists write, When dogs had been trained with reward-based methods, they were more likely to play when the owner was in the room compared to when the stranger was in the room. The dogs in the reward-based group spent more time exploring the room in the presence of the owner compared to when the stranger was there. These dogs also engaged in more greeting behaviours when the owner came back into the room.
“Together with our results, this suggests it is not the reward-based training in itself that generates a secure attachment, but rather the aversive-based training that may be related to the absence of a secure-base effect.”
There were no differences between the two groups of dogs in terms of the two other aspects of attachment, separation distress or maintaining contact with the owner.
It’s worth noting that there were some order effects in this study, and some of the significant results only applied to one of the orders. This is likely because in one order the dog had one, long separation from the owner, compared to the other design which had three, shorter separations.
34 dogs took part in the study. Half had attended one of three reward-based training schools, and half had attended one of three aversive-based dog training schools, for a period of at least 2 months.
It makes sense that dogs trained with aversive methods are more likely to have an insecure attachment, because the same has been found with children when positive punishment is used to discipline them.
Although more research is needed, this is a well-designed study. The data is correlational (and so does not prove causation), but the authors say it is difficult to do randomized controlled trials in this area because of ethical concerns with assigning people’s dogs to aversive training methods when they are associated with stress.
The authors also point out that different kinds of people might choose different dog training methods and training schools, and say that more research on how and why people make these choices would be welcome. This is something I have also argued for (see: why don’t more people use positive reinforcement to train dogs?). Other research has found that men with depression are more likely to use aversive dog training methods.
It’s worth noting that there were some order effects in this study, and some of the significant results only applied to one of the orders. This is likely because in one order the dog had one, long separation from the owner, compared to the other design which had three, shorter separations.
34 dogs took part in the study. Half had attended one of three reward-based training schools, and half had attended one of three aversive-based dog training schools, for a period of at least 2 months.
It makes sense that dogs trained with aversive methods are more likely to have an insecure attachment, because the same has been found with children when positive punishment is used to discipline them.
Although more research is needed, this is a well-designed study. The data is correlational (and so does not prove causation), but the authors say it is difficult to do randomized controlled trials in this area because of ethical concerns with assigning people’s dogs to aversive training methods when they are associated with stress.
The authors also point out that different kinds of people might choose different dog training methods and training schools, and say that more research on how and why people make these choices would be welcome. This is something I have also argued for (see: why don’t more people use positive reinforcement to train dogs?). Other research has found that men with depression are more likely to use aversive dog training methods.